The Meaning of Meanness: Popularity, Competition, and Conflict among Junior High School Girls Don E. Merten Ball State University The “dirty dozen” was the name used by several teachers to refer to a clique of junior high school girls who were both mean and popular. In this school, the students used the term mean as a largely undifferentia­ ted characterization for acts of commission and omission whose intent, or result, was to hurt someone emotionally. This article proposes that exploring the meaning of meanness is a starting point for understanding the connections between female competition, conflict, and popularity. An examination of these connections in the context of a clique of popular girls allows for a better understanding of the sociocultural construction of meanness in junior high school. he sociocultural construction of meanness was a byproduct of compe­ meanness among a clique of pop­ tition and conflict, but at other times, ular girls in junior high school is girls used meanness instrumentally to the focal point of this article. The term gain a competitive advantage in pursuit sociocultural is used here to designate or protection of popularity. Yet it was the interplay of social and cultural phe­ not obvious why being mean seemed nomena in the construction of mean­ reasonable to these girls—much less ness (Berger and Luckmann 1967; why they took meanness to the point of Geertz 1973; Searle 1995). In the con­ being considered the meanest girls in text of the research presented here, the school. construction was explored primarily by examining how the social relationships, PREVIOUS RESEARCH and their meanings, of junior high The nature of competition and con­ school girls were shaped by the broader contours of mainstream American cul­ flict among females has drawn the ture. Therefore, it considered how attention of some investigators because meanness acquired meaning through it was suspected that girls were not suf­ (1) its relationship to other related con­ ficiently competitive to prepare them cepts, such as “niceness”; (2) the mean­ for successful careers (Lever 1976). ing of competition and conflict for girls; Early research on. female competition and (3) the tension between hierarchy compared it, often implicitly, to male and equality. Thus, the construction of forms of competition (Lever 1976) and meanness involved both social interac­ generally found that females were less tion and cultural meaning—the latter competitive (Gilligan 1982; Goodwin 1980). For example, Miller, Danaher, often tacit. For the clique of popular girls whose and Forbes (1986:544) found that fe­ actions are the focus of this article, males could be expected to “use tactics meanness became an essential feature that diffuse conflict or otherwise try of their competition for, and conflict and maintain interpersonal harmony in over, popularity. The relationship the face of conflict.” Moreover, Lever among competition, conflict, and mean­ (1976) suggested that girls were ness was far from simple. Sometimes, sufficiently concerned that competition T 176 would lead to conflict that they would abandon their game, rather than become embroiled in conflict. This was espe­ cially the case when the competition or conflict occurred in an intimate group (Gilligan 1982). These avoidance pat­ terns were shaped by tacit cultural understandings that discouraged open competition among females (Becker 1987; Longino and Miner 1987). More recently, however, female competition has come to be understood neither as a lesser version of male competition nor as something girls invariably avoid but, rather, as competition that is different in its process and meaning (Hughes 1988). This alternative perspective on com­ petition among girls is represented by Hughes’s (1988) description of the social construction of “meanness,” “niceness,” and “nice-mean” in the game of Foursquare. Girls competed in a manner that took into account their social relationships, especially their commitment to their friends. Whereas a player who was removed from the game, as an “out,” may have felt that she was being treated meanly, the person getting her out could mitigate this expe­ rience by suggesting that she was sorry or could not avoid getting her out. In such instances, it was not “really mean” to get someone out, especially when doing so was necessary to get, or keep, one’s friends in the game. Even though the rules of the game constrained a play­ er’s options, they did not, as Hughes pointed out, dictate the meaning of cer­ tain actions. The meanings were con­ structed and negotiated in the local con­ text, especially by mediating “mean” versus “nice” with the idea of “nicemean.” The situation described in the pre­ sent article is both similar to and differ­ ent from the game situation described by Hughes (1988). An obvious similari­ ty is that members of the clique were competing to become or to remain pop­ ular. Competition for popularity was a nearly ubiquitous concern for these junior high school girls. In addition, like the situation Hughes (1983) de­ scribed, the competitive process was Merten inextricably linked to relationships with peers insofar as friends entered competition for popularity as both rivals and supporters. However, there were also important differences between this study and Hughes’s. For one, what members of the clique were competing for, popularity, was less objectified than the outcomes of a game and more encompassing with regard to their sense of self. Moreover, the mem­ bers were not competing in a game with clearly defined (though variable) rules and procedures. One can ask whether the need to mediate nongame competi­ tion was as compelling as for games or whether nongame situations forced girls to compete even more covertly because of the “taboo” on overt competition (Tracy 1991). In other words, were girls discouraged from acknowledging their competition even when competition for popularity was pervasive (Longino and Miner 1987)? In a similar vein, Sheldon (1992) described linguistic means that girls used to mediate conflict. Sheldon’s framework distinguished between “double-voiced” and “single-voiced” discourse. The former was found to occur in solidarity-based groups as indi­ viduals tried to have their way in a con­ flict situation without promoting inter­ personal disharmony. Hence, double­ voiced discourse both pressed an indi­ vidual’s interests and took the other person into account in doing so, thereby preserving interpersonal ties in conflict situations. In short, Sheldon (1992) and Hughes (1983, 1988) both described how girls managed to dull the interper­ sonal sharp edges of their actions by mediating competition or conflict.1 To fail to mediate competition and conflict, as members of the clique often did, was perceived as taking an overtly aggressive stance toward interaction with peers. Because female competi­ tion-conflict that was not mediated was considered mean, one might expect that girls who aggressively pursued it would be considered not only mean but unpopular. That this was not the case with the girls in this clique has already been noted, but these girls were not The Meaning of Meanness unique in this regard. McGuire (1973) found that aggressive girls tended to be popular, rather than unpopular, and Viemero (1992) argued that girls had to be aggressive to be popular. Adler, Kless, and Adler (1992) also described instances in which the mean actions of popular girls were noted by their peers. Yet why the relationship between meanness and popularity exists is unclear. To understand why girls engaged in unmediated competition­ conflict that made them seem aggressive and mean, it is necessary to consider both the group dynamics (Healy and Bell 1990; Rabbie, Goldenbeld, and Lodewijkx 1992) and the larger sociocultural themes of the community and junior high school in which mean­ ness occurred.2 The task of the research was to exam­ ine the sociocultural construction of meanness, particularly how competi­ tion for popularity led junior high school girls to be openly mean. The meaning of meanness was not merely its definition but, rather, the combi­ nation of social and cultural factors that led girls to undertake certain actions that their peers identified as mean. Girls’ accounts of meanness conveyed how they experienced it and included both explicit reasons why people were mean and allusions to unstated reasons for meanness. Therefore, they provided an opening to the meanings of mean­ ness in this junior high school clique. The sociocultural construction of mean­ ness, from the perspective taken here, was interrelated with the construction of popularity, the transformability of popularity into power, and the feelings of invulnerability and vulnerability that accompanied high levels of popularity. Yet one must also consider the broader context in which meanness occurred. For example, where were the adults when all this was going on: How did teachers and parents respond to the inordinate meanness of the clique? Moreover, was there something about this community or school culture that allowed or even encouraged meanness? 177 METHOD AND CONTEXT The data for this article are from a three-year longitudinal study of junior high school students. The first year was spent observing and interviewing stu­ dents in the junior high school. Data from the initial observations and inter­ views (precohort) were used to orient research for the study of the student cohort that entered junior high school the following year. All students who wanted to participate and who had signed informed-consent letters (270 students, 127 boys and 143 girls, 80 per­ cent of the eligible students) formed the study cohort. During the seventh and eighth grades, two school ethnographers observed and interviewed the cohort students at school. A third ethnograph­ er interviewed the parents and adults in the community. These three female ethnographers received intensive training in interview­ ing and observational techniques. They were supervised weekly by two senior ethnographers, one of whom was this author. The two school ethnographers had access to classrooms, halls, the lunchroom, activities, and school events. They interviewed students in a small room designated for that purpose. The interviews were audiotaped and later transcribed, as were the observa­ tional notes. The questions students were asked sought to elicit how they perceived and experienced their world. Most students were interviewed several times and some as many as a dozen times. The community in which the junior high school was located was a middleto upper-middle-class suburb that was overwhelmingly White but was ethnically relatively diverse. It was a community with a heavy emphasis on mobility, both geographic and econom­ ic. The adults in the community were also aware that it was not getting any easier to succeed and that children would have to work hard to do as well as their parents—much less surpass them. Community and family resources were expended to create an educational and activity environment that provided Notice to Contributors Editorial Procedures Al! papers considered appropriate for this journal are reviewed anonymously. To ensure anonymity, authors’ names, institutional affiliations, and other identifying material should be placed on the title page only. Papers are accepted for publication subject to nonsubstantive, stylistic editing. A copy of the edited paper is sent to the author for final review. Proofs of articles are sent only to authors who reside in the United States. Submission of a paper to a professional journal is considered an indication of the author’s commitment to publish in that journal. /X paper submitted to this journal while it is under review for another journal will not be accepted for review. Preparation and Submission of Manuscripts. 1. Type all copy (including indented material, references, and footnotes) double-spaced in no smaller than 10-point type using l'/2-inch margins on all sides. 2. Type each table on a separate page. Insert a note in the text indicating where the table should appear. 3. On an article's acceptance, submit camera-ready art for all figures, rendered on a laser printer or as glossy prints. 4. Include an abstract of no more than 100 words. 5. Submit four copies of the paper and retain the original for your files. Enclose a stamped, self­ addressed postcard so we can acknowledge receipt of your paper. 6. A check or money order for $15.00, payable to the American Sociological Association, must accom­ pany each submission. This fee is waived for papers written by student members of the ASA. The submission fee reflects a policy of the ASA Council and Committee on Publications, which affects all ASA journals. It a reluctant response to the accelerating costs of manuscript processing. Reference Format 1. In the text: All references to books, articles, and other works should be identified at the appropriate point in the text by the surname of the author and year of publication; add page numbers only when citing statistics or direct quotes. Footnotes should be used only for substantive observations and explanations. Subsequent citations of a source should be identified in the same way; do not use "ibid.;' “op. cit.,” or "loc. cit.” a. If the author’s name is part of the narrative, place only the year of publication in parentheses: Duncan (1959). Otherwise, place both the name and the year, with no intervening punctuation, in parentheses: (Duncan 1959). b. Insert page numbers, preceded by a colon after the year of publication: (Kuhn 1970: 120-45). c. If the work cited has five or fewer authors, list all authors in the first citation; thereafter, include only the name of the first author followed by “et al.” If the work has six or more authors, include only the name of the first author followed by “et al.” in all citations. d. Abbreviate or shorten the names of institutional or corporate authors, making sure that the text citation and the entry in the reference list begin with the same element. e. Distinguish two or more works by the same author with the same publication date by appending letters (a, b, c) to the date: (Levy 1965a). f. Separate a series of references with semicolons and enclose them in a single pair of parentheses: (Featherman and Hauser 1979; Coleman et al. 1982; U.S. Bureau of the Census 1981). 2. In the Reference List: List all entries alphabetically by author and, within author, by year of publication. List all authors in citations of multiauthor works; do not use “et al.” in the reference list. Examples follow: Bourdieu, Pierre, 1977. “Cultural Reproduction and Social Reproduction." Pp. 487-511 in Power and Ideology in Education, edited by J. Karabel and A. H. Halsey. New York: Oxford University Press. Coleman, James S., Thomas Hoffer, and Sally B. Kilgore. 1982a. “Cognitive Outcomes in Public and Private Schools.” Sociology of Education 55:65-76. -------- . 1982b. High School Achievement: Public, Catholic and Other Private Schools Compared. New York: Basic. Mare, Robert D. 1979. "Change and Stability in Educational Stratification.” Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Sociological Association, Boston. Marx, Karl (1867) 1976. Capital. Vol. 1. Translated by S. Moore and E. Aveling. New York: International. U.S. Bureau of the Census. 1979. 1970 Census of Population and Housing. Fourth Count Population Summary Tape. Machine-readable data file. Washington, DC: U.S. Bureau of the Census (pro­ ducer). Rosslyn, VA: DUALabs (distributor).